1/26 Blog Post (Joey
Tedeschi)
In the first chapter of Changing Minds, diSessa delves into
unpacking the term literacy, and what it means in a traditional way versus what
computational literacy might mean.
diSessa goes on to talk about the three pillars of literacy: material,
mental (cognitive), and social. The
material pillar of literacy includes understanding that literacy involves
external symbols, signs, depictions and representations. She mentions the vast range of inscription
forms computer technology offers as well (spreadsheets, pictures, game
interfaces, CD’s, DVD’s, etc.). The mental pillar suggests that the material
pillar would be useless without a way to think and interact with inscriptions
via our minds. The social pillar
suggests literacy is very socially constructed and certain types of
inscriptions are only implemented into society if there is a great need or
benefit for them. diSessa then goes on
to talk about Galileo and how the addition of algebra into our society (a new
inscription system) has helped us understand calculus by simplifying (and
building on) what Galileo had written. diSesa then touches on more social
niches in regards to literacy.
Nersessian’s
research paper focused on modeling practices in an ethnographic study of a
neural engineering research lab. The
paper really highlighted the dynamic and social nature of the model-based
reasoning process. The conceptual innovation
observed throughout the paper stems from many different models (conceptual,
physical, and computational) developed by a team of unique scientists. The team wanted to create a simulation device
in vitro that can serve as a very good model to predict certain phenomena in
vivo (specifically dealing with neurons).
Along the way to creating this device, there were many different phases
of investigation, models created, social interactions, collaboration,
revisions, and analyses involved. The
paper gave a great overview of how scientific practices are implemented in the
real scientific community.
Going back to the
NGSS framework, there were 8 practices deemed essential for the K-12 science
and engineering curriculum. These
included:
1. Asking questions (for science) and defining
problems (for engineering)
2. Developing and using models
3. Planning and carrying out investigations
4. Analyzing and interpreting data
5. Using mathematics and computational thinking
6. Constructing explanations (for science) and
designing solutions (for
engineering)
7. Engaging in argument from evidence
8. Obtaining, evaluating, and communicating
information
I
would say that in Nersessian’s paper every single one of these practices were prominently
involved throughout the research. The
researchers had a question about neurons and defined a problem to solve. How can we use technology to build and better
understand “a living neural network”?
They developed multiple models (conceptual, physical, computational) to
help represent the phenomena and system in question. They planned and carried out many
investigations, analyzed the data and made tweaks to fix problems. Math and computational thinking were involved
in the construction of models and designing solutions. The researchers were constantly constructing explanations
and designing solutions. Along the way
they would use data and other resources to argue using evidence. As many different researchers came together
to complete the solution; they obtained, evaluated, and communicated
information to each other along the way.
Also, by having this research published, this information was
communicated to me as well.
I
would say that diSessa was most focused on the practice of communicating information,
specifically about literacy and its different aspects. However, in this chapter she asked questions
in regards to literacy, planned and carried out an investigation into Galileo’s
theories, and constructed explanations for what he meant and how a modern
reader might interpret Galileo’s writing.
Math and computational thinking was definitely involved when she
reproduced Galileo’s proof of Theorem 1 with algebra. She engaged in argumentation by giving
evidence of how a new inscription system can drastically change how we look at
things. She did this by using Galileo
and how algebra has changed our interaction with calculus. She also comments on how important systematic
representational systems can be in aiding discovery (by allowing abstract patterns
be converted into spatial, visible ones).
Although
the two readings were very different in regards to content (neuroscience
research vs. literacy discussion), I found it very intriguing that both of the
authors used or referenced the 8 practices in one-way or another. This really shows how dynamic these practices
can be!
I agree that the Nersessian article hit more of the 8 practices than diSessa, but I thought that the practice of modeling was featured most prominently. However, I was left wondering about how this process is started. How do we start to build a model that we really don’t know anything about? If we couldn’t really measure the bursts effectively in vitro, then how could we tell that our model is displaying the same behavior. It’s something I hope we discuss further.
ReplyDeleteI agree that the Nersessian article showcases many more of the NGSS practices than diSessa's piece, and I think that is because of the nature of the piece. Nersessian's argument is based on an ethnographic study of laboratory researchers, so we would expect that they are naturally doing the practices that the NGSS expect students to engage in. But, she elaborates most on the modeling activities and reasoning that the scientists did because those are most crucial to her argument. diSessa's argument is a bit different because she's looking at representation systems as literacy, and is trying to convince her readers to view programming as a new literacy because of its affordances for modeling.
ReplyDelete